Home / Journal / Interdisciplinary Education and Psychology
achievement motivation student-teachers relationships school transition
Roch Chouinard, Normand Roy, Isabelle Archambault, and Jonathan Smith
DOI: https://doi.org/10.31532/InterdiscipEducPsychol.2.1.001 28 Nov 2017
The present study examined the link between students’ relationships with teachers and achievement motivation in the context of the transition to secondary school. Participants were 323 French-speaking students from various public schools of the Province of Quebec (Canada). A self-reported questionnaire measuring students’ appreciation of their relationships with teachers as well as their competence beliefs, utility value, interest, and achievement goals (mastery, performance-approach and work-avoidance) was administered to participants immediately before and after their transition to secondary school. Growth curve analyses indicate that, following the transition to secondary school, participants experienced a decline of achievement motivation. Results further show that students’ achievement motivation co-varied with their relationships with teachers, before and after the transition to secondary school. For instance, students’ who improved their relationships with teachers following this transition also reported a lower motivational decrease. These results highlight the need for schools to emphasize students’ relationships with their teachers in order to facilitate the transition to secondary school.
Achievement motivation, student-teachers, relationships, school transition
Motivation is widely known to be associated with student engagement and school success (Bandura, 1997, Hardré, 2015; Wigfield, Cambria, & Eccles, 2012). Many researchers have documented a reduction in students’ achievement motivation across the school years (Chouinard & Roy, 2008; Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002; Watt, 2004), and especially after the transition to secondary school (Dotterer, McHale, & Crouter, 2009; Duchesne, Ratelle, & Feng, 2014; Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 2001; Zanobini & Usai, 2002). However, although research generally underlines that the transition from elementary to secondary school is deemed to be a stressful event that can undermine students' motivation, most of these studies also stated that some students do not seem to be affected that much (Jozsa & Morgan, 2014; Ratelle, Guay, Larose, & Senécal, 2004; Tonkin & Watt, 2003). How can we explain these differences? Studies did not provide a valid answer to this question yet, but the quality of the relationships students share with their teachers during that period might be part of the answer.
Motivation is a key factor in students’ learning process, which contributes to their academic achievement. This is well-recognized in established theoretical models, such as the Expectancy-Value theory (Wigfield, Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-Kean) and the Achievement Goals theory (Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1992; Nicholls, 1984).
Inspired by the socio-cognitive approach, the Expectancy-Value theory has been widely used over the last decades to conceptualize the development of achievement motivation. According to the modern version of this theory, engagement and achievement are best predicted by the combination of students’ expectations of success and subjective task value (Wigfield et al.; 2015; Wigfield et al, 2009). The Expectancy component corresponds to the beliefs students hold about how well they will perform on an upcoming task and relates to their perception of being able to carry out their academic projects successfully. Expectancy appears to be influenced by specific beliefs such as competence beliefs which refer to the assessment done by the children themselves of their skills and capability to perform well in a specific domain (Eccles et al., 1993). The Value component refers to the intrinsic significance, utility, attainment value, and cost students ascribed to a given task, a school subject or school in general (Wigfield, Muensks, & Rosenzweig, 2015). On one side, intrinsic value refers to interest and immediate enjoyment derived from engagement, while attainment value is the importance of tasks and academic achievement for the individual. On the other side, utility value concerns how a task or a student’s academic achievement is related to his current and future goals. Finally, cost is defined as the negative aspects of engaging in a task, such as pessimistic emotions and effort (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). In the present study, we investigated how students change across the transition to secondary school along three of the most central components of the Expectancy-Value theory: competence beliefs, utility value and interest.
Goals theory suggested originally the existence of two general goal orientations: mastery and performance goals (Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1992; Nicholls, 1984). Students presenting a mastery goals orientation place great importance on learning and understanding, whereas students presenting a performance goals orientation focus on demonstrating their competence in relation to others (Elliot, Murayama, & Pekrun, 2011). Nevertheless, some researchers have produced evidence stating students’ performance goals may differ according to their self-perceptions (Bouffard, Boisvert, Vezeau, & Larouche, 1995). As a result, many authors now view achievement goals theory from an approach-avoidance viewpoint (Harackiewicz, Barron, Pintrich, Elliot & Trash, 2002; Wigfield et al., 2015). In this line, several authors suggest that for some students, the ultimate goal is to invest the least amount of effort in learning (Bouffard et al., 1998; Meece & Holt, 1993). Therefore, these authors propose a three-dimension achievement goals model comprising mastery (approach) goals, performance (approach) goals, and work avoidance goals. This three-dimension model is the one retained for the present study.
Changes in Achievement Motivation across the Transition to Secondary School Research has demonstrated that there are significant changes in children’s achievement motivation over time (Fredricks & Eccles, 2002; Watt, 2004; Wigfield et al., 2015). According to a large number of studies, many students appear to become less optimistic over time with respect to their chances of succeeding and show a significant decline in their competence beliefs (Jacobs et al., 2002; Watt, 2004; Wigfield et al., 2006). At the same time, the intrinsic value they assent to different academic domains declines as adolescents increasingly view school as less interesting, significant, and useful (Jacobs et al., 2002). However, it seems that not all children experience the same achievement motivation decline of achievement motivation over time. For example, Archambault, Eccles, and Vida (2010) identified seven different trajectories of student task beliefs (self-concept and task value in literacy) between grade 1 and 12. They showed that most students present a general decline in those beliefs over time, but to different degrees according to their profile. Studies that have examined changes in achievement goals during adolescence are limited. Still, available results do indicate that achievement goals remain mostly stable in elementary school (Meece & Miller, 2001), while there would be a decrease in mastery goals orientation (Fischer & Theis, 2014; Paulick, Watermann, & Nückles, 2013; Shim, Ryan, & Anderson, 2008) and an increase in avoidance and performance goals during early adolescence (Bouffard, Boileau, & Vezeau, 2001; Chouinard & Roy, 2008). This drop in students’ self-perceptions, competence beliefs, interest for school and intrinsic motivation has particularly been documented across the transition to secondary school (Benner, 2011; Cantin & Boivin, 2004; Dotterer, McHale, & Crouter, 2009; Zanobini & Usai, 2002).
A variety of explanations have been proposed to explain the decline in motivation after the transition from elementary to secondary school. First, it is tempting to say that the concepts taught in secondary school are more difficult compared to the 6th grade curriculum. Accordingly, these new difficulties may explain the overall decline in student competence beliefs and interest. However, there is little evidence to support that assumption (Anderson, et al., 2000). Others state that the phenomenon could be imputed mainly to physiological and psychological changes affecting students at this stage (new interests, enlarged views, lassitude, etc.) (Simmons & Blyth, 1987). Yet, this claim is not well supported and some authors have pinpointed that many students still encounter difficulties even when their transition occurs in ninth grade instead of seventh grade (Neild, 2008; Smith, 2006).
Other explanations associate the decline in students’ motivation to organizational changes occurring between elementary and secondary schools. Some authors suggest that the secondary school environment is more impersonal, competitive, and formal compared to the elementary school setting (Eccles et al. 1993; Coffey, 2013). Secondary schools are larger and more departmentalized, classroom management is more authority-oriented, student-teacher ratios are higher, and lessons are more often given in collective format (Eccles & Roeser, 2009; Roeser, Urdan, & Stephens, 2009). As such, it is proposed that the gradual drop in students’ motivation during the transition to secondary school could be imputed to the lack of concordance between secondary school organization and the psychological needs of competence, connectedness, and autonomy of adolescents. This explanation is grounded in the stage-environment fit theory (Eccles et al., 1993) which suggests that the secondary school context does not allow teenagers to satisfy the psychological needs corresponding to their stage of development. As a result, using control-oriented discipline and competitive academic values with early adolescents who psychologically seek for more autonomy would tends to produce a mismatch between context and students’ developmental needs which, in turn, would be associated with lower levels of motivation (Pianta, 2006). The more formal environment of secondary schools could jeopardize students’ connectedness with school, by lowering the quality of the relationships with teachers.
Relationships with teachers Effective teachers are expected to be caring, supportive, and emotionally close to each of their students while being responsive to their personal, social, and academic needs (Wentzel, 2015, Coffey, 2013). They also promote democratic and respectful interactions, set expectations for performance based on individual differences, provide constructive feedback and become increasingly critical in the regulation of emotions experienced in the classroom (Pianta, 2006; Wentzel, 2015). Supporting this idea, research shows that students who feel respected and valued by their teachers are more engaged and make more efforts in the classroom (Roorda, Koomen, Spilt, & Oort, 2011; Wang & Eccles, 2012). Moreover, perceived warmth and support from teachers have been related to important motivational factors, including mastery goals, interest, utility value, and competence beliefs (Stipek, 2002; Wang & Holcombe, 2010). The major role on students’ achievement motivation is easy to comprehend given the fact that teachers are the first judges of students' skills and that they constantly reflect back to students’ judgments on their competence and self-worth, as well as on the utility and value of learning (Patrick, Turner, Meyer, & Midgley, 2003).
Many authors have emphasized the lower quality of relationships with teachers in secondary school compared to elementary school. For instance, after the transition to secondary school, students would experience a decrease in quantitative and qualitative contact with teachers who trust them less (Eccles & Roeser, 2009) and with whom their share more conflict (Roderick, 1993). Teachers' classroom management also becomes more controlling, which is in opposition with the increased need of autonomy of adolescents (Eccles & Roeser, 2009). Because teachers in secondary schools often teach many students per day, the organizational structure of their work does not facilitate the development of close relationships with students and may instead foster feelings of isolation in the latter (Juvonen, 2007). Relationships with teachers thus become more impersonal at a time when adolescents increasingly need the support of adults other than their parents (Pianta, 2006).
Aim of the present study The negative changes observed after the transition to secondary school in students’ relationships with teachers and achievement motivation are well documented. However, there is still much work to do before we fully understand whether these changes can be related. Since the declines that occur in students’ relationships with teachers and achievement motivation after the transition have mostly been studied separately, the associations between these changes are not well documented (Wang & Eccles, 2012). In order to address this limitation of previous research, the aim of the present study was to examine how the relationships that girls and boys share with their teachers and how the changes in these relationships are associated with achievement motivation across the transition to secondary school. To attain this goal, we examined the connection between participants’ relationships with their teachers and central motivational components: competence beliefs, utility value, interest and achievement goals (mastery, performance-approach, work avoidance) before the transition and at the beginning and the end of the transition year. Gender was also used as a control variable.
The present study is based on a sample of French speaking students from the Province of Quebec (Canada). We assessed these students’ achievement motivation and the quality of their relationships with teachers using a three-point longitudinal design. This design allowed us to answer our research questions by studying the changes and associations between our different variables at the end of 6th grade (T1), through the beginning (T2) and the end (T3) of Secondary 1. In Quebec schools, 6th grade represents the last year of elementary school in Quebec while Secondary 1 corresponds to the first of the five years of secondary level education. Like elsewhere, there are noteworthy differences between primary and secondary schools in the province. To mention the most important one, secondary schools are usually larger and have an extended curriculum. Secondary school students are also generally ascribed to different specialists, in groups averaging 30 students. Conversely, primary school students are ascribed to one generalist teacher for basic subjects and to several specialists for other subjects (arts, second language, physical education, etc.) in classes averaging 25 students.
Participants and Procedure At the outset of the study (end of sixth grade), 517 sixth grade students (278 females; mean age 12.18 years, σ =.68) from 17 French public elementary schools were recruited to participate in the research. The following year, 323 of these participants (170 females; mean age at the beginning of the study = 12.83, σ =.70) were followed in the public secondary school of their neighborhood (12 schools). Most of the other participants that were lost to the present study transferred to different private secondary schools, which is a common practice in Quebec where public and private institutions are in competition. On average, 20% of students are educated in the private secondary school system of the province. Nevertheless, results from a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) indicate no significant differences on the dependent variables between the group of participants who left the study after sixth grade and those who were followed after the transition to secondary school (F(9, 465) = .612; p = .79).
Instruments Several scales from varied sources were used to collect the data pertaining to the present study. In all cases, participants expressed their opinions on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Totally disagree) to 6 (Totally agree). All scales showed acceptable internal consistency with Cronbach’s alphas equal or over .70 (Gliem & Gliem, 2003).
Competence beliefs, utility value, and interest were measured with adapted scales produced by Ntamakiliro, Monnard, and Gurtner (2000). All three scales comprised four items and assessed academic achievement motivation in general. The competence beliefs scale measured participants’ evaluation of their capacity to do well in school (e.g., “I am as capable as others to achieve in school”) (α = .841). Utility value was based on participants’ perceptions of present or future usefulness of what they learn and do in class (e.g., “What we learn in school will be useful in my future life”) (α = .88). The interest subscale assessed the degree to which participants were interested in their classes (e.g. “What we do in school is really interesting”) (α =.78).
Achievement goals (mastery-approach, performance-approach, and work avoidance) were assessed with scales produced and validated by Harackiewicz, Durik, Barron, Linnenbrink-Garcia, and Tauer (2008). All three scales reveal satisfactory levels of reliability and consistency in the French version of the instrument that was used in the present study. The mastery-approach goals subscale (α = .76) entailed three statements assessing the extent to which participants wished to master the content they learn (e.g., “What is really important for me in school is to learn as much as possible.”). The performance-approach goals subscale (α = .82) consisted of four statements measuring the degree to which participants set as a personal goal to be among the best in their classes and to obtain high marks (e.g., “What is most important for me at school is to achieve better than the other students”). The work avoidance goals subscale (α = .70) was comprised of three statements measuring the degree to which participants set their goals to do the least amount of work possible (e.g., “I always try to work as less as possible in school”).
Relationships with teachers were conceptualized based on Adams and Singh (1998) and Wentzel's (1998) definitions which include warmth, support and lack of conflict. From these definitions, we developed and validated a six items scale (α =.85) using exploratory factor analysis. This scale is composed of two items measuring participants' perception of teacher support (e.g., “My teachers make me feel like I can succeed”) adapted from the Fennema and Sherman’s Mathematics Attitudes Scales (1976). These items were translated in French and validated by Vezeau, Chouinard, Bouffard and Couture (1998). Four other items measuring warmth or lack of conflict in the student-teacher relationships were developed by Janosz, Bouthillier, Bowen, Chouinard and Desbiens (2007) (e.g., “I feel like I am respected by my teachers”; “I often quarrel with my teachers” –this item was reverse coded).
Data Treatment Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM) was used to model motivational changes according to participants’ perception of their relationships with teachers in 6th grade and after the transition to secondary school. HLM techniques are usually used to deal with clustering of students within classrooms or schools. However, in consideration of our research design, where individuals of the same classroom were ascribed to different schools and different classrooms after they were transferred to secondary school, we did not consider the varying hierarchical levels of the data. Instead, as recommended by Woltman, Feldstain, Mackay and Rocchi (2012), we used HLM to account for the heterogeneity of variance that exists across the repeated measures of our research design. As such, HLM analysis technique allowed us to estimate the intercept and the slope for each group and for each variable (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). For clarity and to favor the interpretability of our results, growth models were estimated separately for all achievement motivation outcomes.
Several of the 323 participants that we were able to follow to their secondary school present some missing data. At Times 1 and 2, only a small proportion of missing data was observed (between 1.2% and 5.3%) but at Time 3, that proportion was more important (between 18.9% and 21%). Most students who present missing data were absent from school when data collections were realized at Time 2 and 3. Another main reason for missing data during the same period was the withdrawal from the study of several teachers in the context of union pressure. Data from participants who completed the study to the end (n =243) and those that we were unable to reach at Time 3 (n = 61) were compared at Time 1 and then at Time 2. Results from our analysis (MANOVA) indicate no significant differences between the two groups on the dependent and independent variables (Time 1: F(9, 309) = .663; p = .74; Time 2: F(9, 294) = .714; p = .70). As such, we concluded that the data were missing at random (Graham, 2012), which allowed us to impute these missing data in order to make full use of all available information. To do so, we used the missing data multiple imputations procedure of IBM SPSS Statistics (GradPack 21) and computed model parameters from all available data points. This procedure is highly preferable to other popular methods like listwise and pairwise deletion or mean substitution that might bias results (Garson, 2015).
Preliminary Analysis Firstly, we conducted a preliminary analysis with HLM in order to measure general time effect on relationships with teachers between the end of 6th grade and the beginning and the end of Secondary 1. Results indicate a general and slight but significant, decrease in relationships with teachers between the end of 6th grade and the beginning of Secondary 1 (β = -.19, t (322) = -3.67, p < .001), and a much more considerable decrease between 6th grade and the end of Secondary 1 (β = -.57, t (322) = -9.94, p < .001).
Baseline Model Secondly, in order to document exclusively the evolution of motivation after the transition to secondary school, we measured time effect on each dependent variable (Y) with a baseline model with no interaction variables at level 2. In this model (see equation below), P1 represents the dependent variables at T1, P2 the slope between time 1 and time 2, and P3 the slope between T1 and T3. The resulting regression equation is: Level-1 Y = P1 + P2(T2) + P3(T3) + E Level-2 P1 = B00 P2 = B10 + R1 P3 = B20 + R2
As reported in Table 1 and 2, interest, mastery goals and work avoidance showed significant changes between the end of 6th grade and the two times of measurement in Secondary 1; these changes are negative in the case of interest (-.12 and -.32) and mastery goals (-.12 and -.34) and positive in the case of work avoidance (.09 and .34). For all these variables, the magnitude of the changes observed is larger between T1 and T3 than between T1 and T2. Students’ competence beliefs and utility value also showed a decline over time, but this decline was significant only by the end of Secondary 1 (-.26 and -.21, respectively). Finally, performance-approach goals reflected no significant time effect.
Table 1. Parameter Estimate (standard errors) and Significant Test for Time-Only Model on Expectancy-Value Variables
|
|
Competence Beliefs |
Utility Value |
Interest |
|||
|
|
β(err.) |
t-value |
β(err.) |
t-value |
β(err.) |
t-value |
T1 |
|
4.69 (.06) |
78.07* |
5.38 (.05) |
115.24* |
4.34 (.06) |
74.62* |
T2 |
Slope |
.04 (.05) |
.85 |
-.02 (.04) |
-.47 |
-.12 (.06) |
-2.28* |
T3 |
Slope |
-.26 (.06) |
-4.50* |
-.21 (.05) |
-3.99* |
-.32 (.06) |
-5.28* |
Degrees of freedom for level 1 variable = 966 and = 322 for level 2 variables. * p < .05
Table 2. Parameter Estimate (standard errors) and Significant Test for Time-Only Model on Achievement Goals Variables
|
|
Mastery-Approach |
Performance-Approach |
Work Avoidance |
|||
T1 |
|
5.43 (.04) |
132.48* |
3.38 (.07) |
49.36* |
1.71 (.05) |
33.75* |
T2 |
Slope |
-.12 (.04) |
-2.93* |
-.05 (.06) |
-.78 |
.09 (.05) |
2.03* |
T3 |
Slope |
-.34 (.05) |
-6.19* |
-.10 (.07) |
-1.43 |
.34 (.06) |
6.11* |
Degrees of freedom for level 1 variable = 966 and = 322 for level 2 variables. * p < .05
Interest At the end of 6th grade, participants with better relationships with teachers also reported greater interest in academic tasks (.60). Analyses also indicate that interest significantly varied with the fluctuation of the relationships with teachers in T2 and T3 (.49 and .43 respectively).
Final Model In order to document the moderating effect of gender and relationships with teachers, our final model encompassed four predictive variables: gender, relationships with teachers (RT) at the end of 6th grade (T1), the difference between RT at the beginning of Secondary 1 (T2) and the end of 6th grade (ΔRTT2-T1), as well as the difference between RT at the end of Secondary 1 (T3) and the end of 6th grade (ΔRTT3-T1). The changes in the relationships with teachers, for example RTT2-T1, were calculated by computing the difference between participants score between times (here, score at T2 minus score at T1). As such, a positive value can be interpreted as an improvement of the relationships over time and a negative value as deterioration. The resulting regression equation is: Level 1 Y = P1 + P2(T2) + P3(T3) + E Level-2 (1) P1 = B00 + B01(Gender) + B02(RTT1) (2) P2 = B10 + B11(Gender) + B12(ΔRTT2-T1) + R1 (3) P3 = B20 + B21(Gender) + B22(ΔRTT3 - T1) + R2
In this equation, Y corresponds to the dependent motivational variable and P1 is the initial state of the dependent variable at the end of 6th grade, moderated by RT at T1. P2 and P3 relate respectively to the slope between T1 and T2 and between T1 and T3. However, contrary to the baseline model, these last slopes are moderated by gender and changes of RT (ΔRTT2-T1 for P2 and ΔRTT3 - T1 for P3).
Competence Beliefs As illustrated in Table 3, by the end of 6th grade, participants reporting better relationships with teachers also reported higher competence beliefs (.33). Results further show that participants’ scores on competence beliefs in T2 and T3 varied significantly with the changes in their relationships with teachers, positively if these relationships improved and negatively if they deteriorated (T2 slope = .26; T3 slope = .28).
Utility Value Participants who reported better relationships with teachers at the end of 6th grade also reported higher utility value at the same time (.25). Additionally, results indicate that the changes in utility value significantly fluctuated with the variations of participants’ relationships with their teachers in T2 and T3. For example, participants who reported an amelioration of their relationships also reported a lesser decline or no decline at all of utility value (T2 slope = .24 and T3 slope = .29).
Table 3. Parameter Estimate (standard errors) and Significant Test for Final Model on Expectancy-Value Variables
|
|
Competence Beliefs |
Utility Value |
Interest |
|||
|
|
β(err.) |
t-value |
β(err.) |
t-value |
β(err.) |
t-value |
T1 |
|
4.70 (.09) |
53.88* |
5.29 (.07) |
73.94* |
4.24 (.08) |
54.61* |
Gender |
-.01 (.12) |
-0.05 |
.17 (.09) |
1.87 |
.19 |
1.81 |
|
RTT1 |
.33 (.07) |
4.76* |
.26 (.05) |
4.91* |
.60 |
11.55* |
|
T2 |
Slope |
.10 (.08) |
1.19 |
.11 (.07) |
1.52 |
.02 (.08) |
.27 |
Gender |
-.01 (.10) |
-0.09 |
-.15 (.09) |
-1.77 |
-.10 (.10) |
-1.00 |
|
∆RTT2 - T1 |
.26 (.06) |
4.10* |
.24 (.04) |
5.63* |
.49 (.06) |
8.09* |
|
T3 |
Slope |
.00 (.09 |
-0.04 |
.04 (.08) |
.50 |
.03 (.09) |
.37 |
Gender |
-.18 (.11) |
-1.58 |
-.15 (.10) |
-1.52 |
-.20 (.11) |
-1.86 |
|
∆RTT3 - T1 |
.28 (.06) |
4.53* |
.29 (.05) |
5.43* |
.43 (.05) |
8.05* |
RT = Relationships with teachers Degrees of freedom for Level 1 variables = 960 Degrees of freedom for Level 2 variables = 320 Males = 0, Females = 1 * p < .05
Mastery-Approach Goals As shown in Table 4, participants who reported better relationships with teachers by the end of 6th grade also reported higher mastery goals at the same period (.33). Moreover, analyses show that mastery-approach goals significantly evolved overtime in relation with the changes reported on students’ relationships with their teachers (.34 and .35 respectively).
Table 4.Parameters Estimate (standard errors) and Significant Test for Final Model on Achievement Goals Variables
|
|
Mastery-Approach |
Performance-Approach |
Work Avoidance |
|||
|
|
β(err.) |
t-value |
β(err.) |
t-value |
β(err.) |
t-value |
T1 |
|
5.38 (.06) |
91.19* |
3.66 (.10) |
34.95* |
1.80 (.07) |
25.26* |
Gender |
.09 (.08) |
1.13 |
-.52 (.14) |
-3.75* |
-.17 (.09) |
-1.86 |
|
RTT1 |
.33 (.04) |
7.44* |
.16 (.07) |
2.38* |
-.38 (.05) |
-7.56* |
|
T2 |
Slope |
-.01 (.06) |
-.20 |
-.13 (.10) |
-1.28 |
-.02 (.07) |
-.41 |
Gender |
-.09 (.08) |
-1.14 |
.22 (.13) |
1.74 |
.15 (.09) |
1.65 |
|
∆RTT2 - T1 |
.34 (.05) |
6.76* |
.18 (.06) |
2.92* |
-.23 (.05) |
-4.11* |
|
T3 |
Slope |
-.09 (.08) |
-1.11 |
-.05 (11) |
-.46 |
.16 (.08) |
2.02* |
Gender |
-.10 (.10) |
-.94 |
.14 (.14) |
1.01 |
.06 (.11) |
.52 |
|
∆RTT3 - T1 |
.35 (.05) |
7.14* |
.21 (.06) |
3.84* |
-.26 (.06) |
-4.39* |
RT = Relationships with teachers Degrees of freedom for Level 1 variables = 960 Degrees of freedom for Level 2 variables = 320 Males = 0, Females = 1 * p < .05
Performance-Approach Goals Participants who reported better relationships with teachers by the end of 6th grade also reported higher performance-approach goals at the same period (.16). Overtime, a significant effect of relationships was found (.18 and .21), indicating that students’ performance-approach goals were related with the changes in their relationships with teachers.
Work Avoidance Goals By the end of 6th grade, participants with better relationships with teachers reported lower work avoidance goals (-.38). After the transition to secondary school (T2 and T3), work avoidance goals significantly increased (see Table 2), but those changes were moderated by student-teacher relationships adjustments. Thus, for participants reporting an improvement or a decline of their relationships with teachers between Time 1 and Time 2 as well as between Time 1 and Time 3, we observed reversed changes of work avoidance goals (-.23 and -.26, respectively).
Finally, males (coded 0) and females (coded 1) reported similar results on all dependent variables at the outset of the study, except for performance-approach goals on which males significantly scored higher than females. Otherwise, no significant moderating gender effect was observed at T2 and T3, meaning that females and males reported equivalent changes on the dependent variables across time with no gender-time interaction.
The aim of this two-year longitudinal study was to examine how relationships with teachers are associated with girls’ and boys’ achievement motivation before and after the transition to secondary school. This research is the first that addressed how changes in teacher-student relationships are associated with the changes on students’ expectation of success, value, and achievement goals at different points in time, from the end of 6th grade, through the beginning and the end of Secondary 1. Based on previous research, we put forth the hypothesis that students who experience an improvement of the relationships they share with teachers following the transition to secondary school will present a lower decline of their achievement motivation. Overall, our results support this idea; positive changes in students’ relationships with teachers across the transition were associated with lower decrease in students’ achievement motivation.
Results first indicate that relationships with teachers and achievement motivation co-varied before and after the transition to secondary school and that these variables generally declined following the transition. These findings concur with those of other researchers who have posited that students tend to reevaluate their academic expectancy, value and goals after the transition to secondary school (Bouffard et al., 2001; Fischer & Theis, 2014; Paulick et al., 2013; Shim et al., 2008).
Our results further indicate that students who improved the quality of the relationships with their teachers right after the transition to secondary school and through the end of the transition year tended to present a lesser decline of their academic achievement goals, expectancies and values while the invert tendency was observed for those who experienced a deterioration of their relationships with teachers. Although it is the first time that research assesses how changes in students’ relationships with teachers influence changes in their achievement motivation, this finding is in line with previous work showing that the quality of the relationships with teachers represents a very important predictor of students’ motivation in school (Grolnick et al., 2002; Opdenakker, Maulana, & den Brok, 2012; Pianta, Hamre, & Allen, 2012; Wentzel, 2015). Through their interactions with students, teachers send them important messages about their self-worth as well as about the value and significance of learning (Patrick et al., 2003; Wentzel, 2014). In doing so, they continuously lead students to reevaluate their skills and goals, and when the relationships with the students are harmonious, the messages that teachers give them seem to contribute to their motivation to achieve. In our own results, scores on motivational variables were significantly higher for students’ who reported an improvement of the relationships with teachers than for their counterparts who experienced a decrease in these relationships. Such results support the hypothesis that changes in the relationships with teachers during the transition can act as a risk or a protective factor, depending on the student's perception of the quality of these relationships.
That being said, the overall deterioration of students’ relationships with teachers and achievement motivation also supports the stage-environment fit theory that posits that secondary school might not properly meet the relational needs of young adolescents and, therefore, might be less beneficial than elementary school for student achievement motivation (Eccles et al., 1993). These findings highlight the link between relationships with teachers and achievement motivation underlined by other researchers (Faircloth & Hamm, 2005; Ibañez, Kuperminc, Jurkovic, & Perilla, 2004; McMahon, Wernsman, & Rose, 2009; Nelson & DeBacker, 2008). That also coincide with the observation that positive connectedness with teachers could be a factor favoring a smooth transition and may eventually be an indicator of the quality of the adaptation after this transition (Akos, 2006; Akos & Galassi, 2004).
The methodological limitations of the present investigation include the following concerns. Firstly, the analyses used in the treatment of the data are correlational in nature, and thus, causal relations cannot be inferred. Secondly, because we used a convenience sample and we lost the participants who transferred to private secondary schools after 6th grade, our results may only apply to public secondary school students, which limits the generalizability of our findings. As such, in future work, it would be interesting to include a subsample of students from private schools. Finally, the methodological approach model used focused on a quantitative design only. A mixed approach model including observations or interviews of students would better adjust with the aim of the study.
In conclusion, many researchers have criticized secondary school for not doing enough to help students experiment positive relationships with teachers, which would contribute to the satisfaction of their connectedness needs (Certo, Cauley, & Chafin, 2003; Newman Kingery, Erdley, & Marshall, 2011; Osterman, 2000). Nevertheless, few studies have examined the link between achievement motivation and the quality of student-teacher relationships in the context of the transition to secondary school. The results of the present study indicate that the quality of the changes in students’ relationships with teachers is linked to the changes in achievement motivation at the outset of secondary education. In our opinion, this challenges the actual organization of secondary school environments and advocate for adjustments to make them more suitable to the connectedness need of adolescents.
Notes 1. All internal consistency measures come from the sample of the present study. 2. At the beginning of Secondary 1, we observed that 48% of the participants reported a deterioration of the relationships with their teachers, 37% stated an improvement and 15% reported no change at all. Between the end of 6th grade and the end of Secondary 1, 66% of them reported a decline, 23% an improvement and 11% reported no change in their relationships with teachers.
This work was supported by the Fonds de recherche du Québec and Ministère de l’Éducation et de l’enseignement supérieur du Québec through the program Réussite et persévérance scolaire. Grant no: R0019096.
The authors would like to thank the principals, teachers, students, and parents of the cooperating schools for their participation.
© 2024. Rivera Publications, Inc. All rights reserved.